University of Rochester
EMERGENCY INFORMATIONCALENDARDIRECTORYA TO Z INDEXCONTACTGIVINGTEXT ONLY
Cognition

Important Gestures in Learning

Children who ‘talk with their hands’ do better at math, a new Rochester study finds. By Jonathan Sherwood ’04 (MA)

Suppose your math teacher writes the following problem on the board: “9 + 3 + 6 = __ + 6.” How would you approach it?

If you’re like most third-graders, your first response would probably be to think the equal sign indicates that the first three numbers should be totaled up and the answer put into the blank.

bme students HANDY ANSWERS: In her study of third- and fourth-graders, Susan Wagner Cook found that gesturing helped children remember important mathematical concepts.

But an interesting little light bulb goes off when the same kids are asked to explain how they got their answer, according to a new Rochester-led study. As the children walk through the problem, pointing along the way, they discover that the equal sign is doing something different in the problem.

“We’ve known for a while that we use gestures to add information to a conversation even when we’re not entirely clear how that information relates to what we’re saying,” says Susan Wagner Cook, a postdoctoral fellow at the University. “We asked if the reverse could be true; if actively employing gestures when learning helps retain new information.”

In a study published this summer in the journal Cognition, lead author Cook suggests that it’s possible to help children learn difficult concepts by providing gestures as an additional and potent avenue for taking in information. In the study of 84 third- and fourth-graders, kids who were asked to physically gesture at math problems were nearly three times more likely than non-gesturers to remember what they’ve learned.

“My intuition is that gestures enhance learning because they capitalize on our experience acting in the world,” says Cook. “We have a lot of experience learning through interacting with our environment as we grow, and my guess is that gesturing taps into that need to experience.”

In her study, 90 percent of students who had learned algebraic concepts using gestures remembered them three weeks later. Only 33 percent of speech-only students who had learned the concept during instruction later retained the lesson. And perhaps most astonishing of all, 90 percent of students who had learned by gesture alone—no speech at all—recalled what they had been taught.

Cook used a variation on a classic gesturing experiment. When third-graders approach a two-sided algebra equation, such as “9 + 3 + 6 = __ + 6” on a blackboard, they usually try to solve it in the simple way they have always approached math problems.

They tend to think in terms of “the equal sign means put the answer here,” rather than thinking that the equal sign divides the problem into two halves. As a result, children often completely ignore the final “+ 6.”

However, even when children discard that final integer, they will often point to it momentarily as they explain how they attacked the problem. Those children who gestured to the number, even though they may seem to ignore it, are demonstrating that they have a piece of information they can’t reconcile. Previous work has shown that the children with that extra bit of disconnected knowledge are the ones ready to learn, which suggests that perhaps giving children extra information in their gesture could lead to their learning.

Cook divided 84 third- and fourth-graders into three groups. One group expressed the concept verbally without being allowed to use gestures. The second group was allowed to use only gestures and no speech, and the third group employed both. Teachers gave all the children the same instruction, which used both speech and gesture.

After three weeks, the children were given regular in-school math tests. Of those children who had learned to solve the problem correctly, only a third of the speech-only students remembered the principles involved, but that figure rose dramatically for the speech-and-gesture, and the gesture-only group, to 90 percent retention.

Cook plans to look into how gesturing could be implemented effectively in classrooms to make a noticeable improvement in children’s learning.

“Gesturing does have one clear benefit,” Cook adds. “It’s free.”